Journal Information
Vol. 16. Issue 2.
Pages 83-89 (April - June 2018)
Share
Share
Download PDF
More article options
Visits
4344
Vol. 16. Issue 2.
Pages 83-89 (April - June 2018)
Essays and Perspectives
Open Access
Vocal dialects and their implications for bird reintroductions
Visits
4344
Bruno Araujo Martinsa,
Corresponding author
bruno.araujo.bio@gmail.com

Corresponding author.
, Giovanna Soares Romeiro Rodriguesa, Carlos Barros de Araújob,c
a Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ciências Biológicas, Centro de Ciências Exatas e da Natureza, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, João Pessoa, PB 58051-900, Brazil
b Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia e Monitoramento Ambiental, Centro de Ciências Aplicadas e Educação, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, Rio Tinto, PB 58297-000, Brazil
c Instituto Federal de Educação, Ciência e Tecnologia do Rio Grande do Norte, IFRN Campus Ipanguaçu, RN 118, S/N, Povoado Base Física, Zona Rural, Ipanguaçu, RN 59508-000, Brazil
Highlights

  • We investigated geographical variation of the vocalizations of the Cactus Conure.

  • A method for determining the best releasing areas is proposed based on bioacoustics.

  • A multinomial model was used to classify the locality of traffic-seized specimens’ calls among natural populations.

  • The method can reduce vocal differences among released and natural populations, and thus improve reintroduction efficiency.

This item has received

Under a Creative Commons license
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Figures (5)
Show moreShow less
Tables (3)
Table 1. The multinomial regression model selections of flight call localities in relation to call parameters.
Table 2. Confusion matrix showing multinomial analyses of call classification efficiencies.
Table 3. Multinomial regression classification results for captive groups from CETAS.
Show moreShow less
Abstract

Parrot species are a common target of animal trafficking, and the animals recovered from anti-trafficking operations are generally reintroduced into nature. However, these reintroductions usually fail to consider geographical vocal differences that are known to be present in some Parrot species. We investigated patterns of geographical variations in Eupsittula cactorum vocalizations and used those data to infer the geographical origins of recovered birds and thus predict the most appropriate reintroduction sites. We recorded four wild populations in northeastern Brazil (between the western region of Rio Grande do Norte State and northeastern Ceará State), and three groups of captive individuals seized from traffickers. We considered seven acoustic parameters to classify the flight calls of the different native populations and used a multinomial model to classify the recovered animals according to the native populations sampled. Our results indicated the existence of geographical dialects. Individual birds had been released in Quixadá, where local calls are acoustically distinct. Acoustical parameters can provide important clues about the origins of captured individuals as well as reduce acoustical contrasts between released individuals and native populations. The application of this methodology could potentially improve the efficacy of reintroduction efforts, by reducing vocal distances between released individuals and the native population.

Keywords:
Bioacoustics
Bird reintroductions
Geographical dialects
Illegal wildlife trade
Animal trafficking
Conservation
Full Text
Introduction

Vocal learning may be defined as the ability to imitate, with or without modification, the vocalizations of conspecifics (or even other species) (Bradbury and Balsby, 2016). Diverse taxa are capable of vocal learning, ranging from birds to mammals (Sewall et al., 2016). In birds, learned calls seem to be restricted to three groups: songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds (Jarvis et al., 2000). Among the many advantages of learned calls is the possibility of coding individual (Berg et al., 2011) and acquisition of information on populations (Salinas-Melgoza and Wright, 2012; Wright, 1996), as well as vocal labeling (Wanker et al., 2005) – which could allow a bird to address a single individual within a flock (which could have special importance among social birds such as parrots). The learning process is hypothesized to often lead to rapid repertoire changes through cultural evolution (Lachlan and Servedio, 2004) and, although subject to morphological restrictions (Medina-García et al., 2015), learning processes are widely believed to lead to labile acoustical call structures (Vielliard and Silva, 2010).

The vocal abilities of parrots have long captivated the human imagination. In the famous Robinson Crusoe novel (Defoe, 1719), the main character had long “conversations” with a parrot. In fact, the ability to imitate human voices is one of the features that make parrots very desirable as pets (de Araújo, 2011), making parrots’ natural populations susceptible to the pet trade. Habitat destruction and animal trafficking are the main factors threatening parrot conservation throughout the world (Berkunsky et al., 2017; Collar, 2000), and many wild South American parrot species are still being harvested from nature because of the pet trade (Daut et al., 2015; Fernandes-Ferreira et al., 2012; Nobrega Alves et al., 2013).

Many countries, including Brazil, have prohibited the commercialization of wild birds, and have curbed that practice by launching large-scale operations to arrest dealers and recover wild specimens – as exemplified by the efforts of IBAMA (the Brazilian Institute of Environmental and Renewable Natural Resources) in Brazil. Nevertheless, the practice of keeping wild birds represents a deeply-rooted cultural tradition, and the over-harvesting of specimens can cause severe populational reductions (Fernandes-Ferreira et al., 2012). Even thought government operations appear to be helping to change cultural habits and reduce bird harvesting, they produce another serious short-term problem: what should be done with the birds seized from traffickers? Currently, birds are usually sent to Wild Animal Screening Centers (CETAS) and eventually released back into the wild. Because of the difficulty in determining the geographical origins of captured birds (i.e. traffickers who are caught are rarely willing to point out the geographical locations of their harvesting sites or accomplices) individuals are usually released outside their natural ranges. This type of reintroduction can create serious threats to natural populations (Marini and Garcia, 2005), including the risk of establishing a new invasive species, the transmission of diseases and parasites, and the hybridization of otherwise genetically distinct races or subspecies (IUCN, 2000).

Considering this complex background, researchers have been attempting to develop novel methodologies that could determine the origins of recovered birds. Molecular analyses coupled with phylogeographic approaches have shown promising results, but assigning animals to exact geographic locations using molecular data still presents various difficulties. Large portions of the distributions of many species, for example, have not yet been sampled, making it difficult to identify the exact geographic origins of confiscated individuals – and the inferred origins of those confiscated birds could easily change if additional data were incorporated into the data set (Presti et al., 2015). Additionally, as mitochondrial DNA provides no information about the adaptive natures of genetic similarities, it could have little or no direct connection to variations in traits relevant to local adaptations, or to genetic incompatibilities between populations (Fernandes and Caparroz, 2013). Finally, molecular analyses require an invasive procedure that takes a considerable time to be achieved, and although those procedures could provide important information about the genetics of avian taxa, they are not feasible at large scales and with the necessary speed at this time – even though the information gathered would be especially valuable in terms of endangered populations.

Magroski et al. (2017) employed Ecological Niche Models (ENM) to estimate continental distributions of species in isolated patches, using specimen calls to test those ENM, and assign the specimens to appropriate patches. Despite some successes with classifying species with innate calls, the methodology demonstrated serious shortfalls when considering species capable of vocal learning (such as parrots). Wide geographic variations among those species prevented the use of call parameters to infer their broad-scale geographical origins. Those authors observed that populations of species such as the Red-shouldered macaw (Diopsittaca nobilis) that were separated by just a few kilometers could show considerable vocal differences, so that even populations within a given patch (but separated by hundreds of kilometers) would encompass large portions of the achievable vocal variations and mask inter-patch variations (Magroski et al., 2017). The presence of geographic dialects can difficult the reintroduction of released individuals in nature, for the ability of a bird to integrate (i.e. mate) local flocks seems to depend on their ability to acquire the unfamiliar local dialect (Wright and Dahlin, 2017).

Magroski et al. (2017) envisioned a continental analysis, and proposed that intra-patch variations prevented vocal classifications among large-scale patches. In this study, we focus on the existence of small-scale vocal variations and test if these can be used to determine the most suitable place for the release of captured E. cactorum, either by determining the original natural range of the individuals, but also by determining the locality with the smallest vocal divergence between the released individuals and the natural population. As geographical dialects might hinder reintroductions reintroduction (Wright and Dahlin, 2017), the reduction of the acoustical differences among reintroduced and natural populations could help guarantee the efficiency of the process.

Materials and methodsTarget species and sampling

The Cactus Conure (Eupsittula cactorum) is a small (25cm long) psittacid that is very common in the dry northeastern forests of South America. Its distribution encompasses the Caatinga dryland vegetation in the northernmost regions of the states of Piaui, Ceará, and Rio Grande do Norte, throughout Paraíba, Pernambuco, to Bahia, and reaching central Minas Gerais (Forshaw, 2010; Sick, 1997). It forms flocks, as do most psittacid species, which shape their social relationships through complex acoustical communication systems (Guerra et al., 2008). The species is commonly raised as pets in Northeastern Brazil (Alves et al., 2016) and is known to be sold in many localities (Nobrega Alves et al., 2013). In fact, psittacids are among the most confiscated species within Brazil (Destro et al., 2012) and illegal traffic is one of the major problems for its conservation (Berkunsky et al., 2017). Understanding the problems associated with wildlife traffic is crucial if we are to mitigate its negative effects.

Our data consisted of two distinct sets of sound recordings: the first set comprised four wild populations recorded directly in the field; the second set consisted of captive individuals recorded at the Wild Animals Screening Center (CETAS) facility in Fortaleza (CE).

We undertook expeditions to make sound recordings of wild individuals in the field from September/2015 to August/2016. Four distinct populations of Cactus Conure in the northernmost portion of their distribution range were considered, located at: Paramoti (CE); Quixadá (CE); Icapuí (CE); and Serra do Mel (RN) (Fig. 1). The recordings were made between 05:00–08:00 and 15:00–18:00, times when psittacids are most vocally active. All recordings were made using a Tascam-DR05 digital sound recorder coupled to a Shure Beta58a microphone mounted on a 50cm diameter parabola with a 19cm focus (Fig. 2A). All recordings were built with a 48kHz sampling rate and 24-bit resolution.

Fig. 1.

Vocal sampling locations of populations of Cactus Conure (Eupsittula cactorum). 1 – Paramoti (CE); 2 – Quixadá (CE); 3 – Icapuí (CE); and 4 – Areia Branca (RN).

(0.12MB).
Fig. 2.

Recording setups used for Cactus Conure (Eupsittula cactorum) sound recordings. Recordings of wild individuals were made using a parabolic dish (A); captive individuals were recorded at close range with the same cardioid microphon (B).

(0.49MB).

During the sampling period, IBAMA (CETAS) informed us of new arrived groups of recovered Cactus Conures. The specimens were kept together in a large cage, and no information on their geographical origins was available. Captured specimens were placed in small individual cages and recorded using a Tascam-DR05 digital sound recorder coupled to a Shure Beta58a microphone. We kept individuals in visual contact with other captive birds so that they would be stimulated to call; a close-range recording setup was used to reduce background noise (Fig. 2B). We were able to record three different groups of individuals, referred to here as A (11 individuals), B (10 individuals), and C (12 individuals). Following the experiments, the specimens were released at Quixadá, where IBAMA maintains one of its Wild Animal Release Area bases (ASAS).

Sound analysis

Most parrot species exhibit vast vocal repertoires (e.g., de Araújo et al., 2011; de Moura et al., 2011; Fernandez-Juricic et al., 1998), with the flight (contact) call being the most studied vocal component in psittacids (Magroski et al., 2017; Medina-García et al., 2015; Salinas-Melgoza and Wright, 2012; Wright, 1996; Wright et al., 2008). In order to avoid pseudoreplication, only a single flight call of each Cactus Conure was selected from each sound recording (McGregor et al., 1992), based on its signal-to-noise ratio and overlap with acoustical artifacts. All selected calls were run through a high pass filter (300Hz) and normalized to 0dB preceding the analyses. This procedure was necessary to reduce problems arising from comparisons of recordings made at different amplitudes that could reduce the precision of acoustic comparisons (Zollinger et al., 2012).

Selecting which acoustical parameters to be measured is not a simple process, and depends upon the physical structures of the signals to be analyzed. Therefore, before we undertook actual measurements, a small dataset sample was examined to ensure the proper selection of homologous parameters that could be measured throughout the entire dataset. The homologous call structures within the dataset allowed us to divide the call into three portions, from which we selected seven acoustic parameters (Fig. 3): Total duration – DT; duration of the ascending portion – DPA; duration of the descending portion – DPD; dominant frequency – FDOM; maximum fundamental frequency – FMax; minimum fundamental frequency – FMin; frequency band (FMaxFMin). All measurements and spectrograms were built using SoundRuler 0.9.6 software (Gridi-Papp, 2007) with a FFT size of 512 points and 0.40 contrast.

Fig. 3.

Measurement map depicting the acoustical parameters measured for the calls of Cactus Conure (Eupsittula cactorum). Total duration – DT; duration of the ascending portion – DPA; duration of the descending portion – DPD; dominant frequency – FDOM; maximum fundamental frequency – FMax; minimum fundamental frequency; FB – frequency band.

(0.41MB).
Statistical analysis

In order to visually identify any geographic variations among the four populations of Cactus Conure, a PCA was built using acoustic parameters, which we considered a proxy of the acoustical space. To avoid problems arising from different acoustic parameter scales, the PCA was built using a correlation matrix. Since the acoustic space is defined as the set of parameters within the call, the existence of geographical dialects would imply the segregation of the acoustic space used by each of the populations analyzed. Additionally, the relative positions of the acoustical spaces of each of the captive group (A, B, C) in relation to native populations could provide clues about the best places for releasing them. Any acoustic spatial segregation, on the other hand, would indicate that captive individuals had calls distinct from those of sampled natural populations, even though the classification would still indicate the best releasing areas due to reduced vocal differences.

We then built a multinomial log-linear model via neural networks based on the parameters of the four natural populations to classify CETAS captive groups in relation to natural populations. As collinear variables could make it more difficult to detect any effects and reduce the efficiency of multinomial classifications, prior to the analysis we removed variables with high collinearity by calculating the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) within a full multinomial model, and dropping the covariate with the highest VIF. We re-ran the analysis and recalculated VIF values until all VIFs values were smaller than 2 (Zuur et al., 2010), so that all variables included in the model presented low collinearity. The non-collinear parameters list is represented in the full model of Table 1.

Table 1.

The multinomial regression model selections of flight call localities in relation to call parameters.

Model  K  AICc  ΔAICc  Wi 
DPD+BF+FMin  12  169  0.79 
DPD+FDom+BF+FMin  15  172  0.19 
FDom+BF+FMin  12  178  10  0.01 
BF+FMin  179  10 
DPD+BF  191  22 
DPD+FDom+BF  12  195  26 
BF  221  52 
FDom+BF  222  54 
DPD+FMin  273  104 
DPD+FDom+FMin  12  277  108 
DPD  284  115 
DPD+FDom  288  119 
FDom+FMin  306  137 
FMin  307  138 
FDom  332  163 
NULL  332  163 

K, number of estimated parameters; AICc, second-order Akaike's information criteria; ΔAICc, variation of AICc in relation to the best model; Wi, Akaike's weight; NULL, null model; Vocal parameters: duration of the descending portion – DPD; frequency band – BF; dominant frequency – FD; maximum fundamental frequency – FMax; minimum fundamental frequency – Fmin.

We built several multinomial models, accounting for all possible combination of parameters. We selected the models based on the second order Akaike criterion (AICc), by comparing the models to a null model, and selecting those with the lowest ΔAICc. As ΔAICc values lower than 4 indicate support for multiple models (Burnham et al., 2011), we tested the classification efficiency of models with ΔAICc lower than 4. The multinomial models were built using R software (R Core Team, 2015) by implementing the nnet package (Ripley and Venables, 2011); AICc comparison tables were through the implementation of the AICcmodavg package (Mazerolle, 2011).

We first used the best model to predict the original population of the call, and if there was a high degree of call variation regionally, as hypothesized by Magroski and collaborators (2017), we expected call differences to be enough for their proper classifications among the studied populations. In order to examine if the model classification efficiency was better than expected by chance, we built a confusion matrix that expressed the percentages of correct classifications of the calls among patches, and compared them to the correct classification expected by chance through a chi-squared test. Hence, even though we used the second order Akaike criterion (AICc) for model selection, the chi-square test was used to evaluate the efficiency of model classification.

Finally, we used the best multinomial model to classify the vocalizations of captive individuals (A, B, C) among the sampled populations. The classification of captive groups among natural populations would indicate the best possible locations for their release regardless of their geographical origins, for the vocalizations of the released individuals would be the closest (among a limited set of possibilities) to the native vocalizations.

Results

The first two axis of the principal component analysis explained 50.3% of the variation in the acoustical parameters. While some overlap was observed between the Icapuí, Serra do Mel, and Paramonti populations, the Quixadá population showed a very distinct call structure, with considerable segregation from other populations within the acoustical space. Paramonti calls fell between those of Icapuí and Serra do Mel, while the latter two populations appeared to be segregated within the acoustical space. Overall, the PCA supported the idea of high call parameter variability and the presence of vocal dialects within close populations such as Quixadá and Paramonti (∼100km) or Icapuí and Serra do Mel (∼70km).

Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) analysis (Zuur et al., 2010) led to the removal of three collinear variables from the model: the maximum fundamental frequency (FMax), total duration (DT), and the duration of the ascending portion (DPA). Two models obtained a ΔAICc value higher than 4 (Table 1), indicating that both had empirical support. We therefore tested the classification efficiencies of those models based on the percentage of correct call classifications within their original populations. While the first model had a high Akaike weight (Wi), which suggests greater empirical support, the second model was found to be more efficient at classifying the calls. We therefore opted to use the second best model for the analysis, as follows: LocalityDPD+FD+BF+FMin (Table 1).

The multinomial analyses showed different efficiencies in classifying calls of the populations examined. The calls of Quixadá had a perfect classification of 100%, indicating its distinctiveness. Other localities, such as Paramonti, showed lower classification values of 74%. Overall, the multinomial model had a correct classification of 82%, and the calls of all of the populations studied were classified better than expected by chance (Table 2). Our results demonstrate clear geographical differences in vocal repertoires supporting the predictions of Magroski et al. (2017).

Table 2.

Confusion matrix showing multinomial analyses of call classification efficiencies.

Locality  n  Correct  Chi-p 
Icapuí  31  25  81  0.0027 
Paramoti  38  28  74  0.0239 
Quixadá  17  17  100  <0.0001 
Serra do Mel  35  29  83  0.0041 
Total  121  99  82   

n, sample size; Correct, the number of correct classifications; %, the percentage of correct classifications; Chi-p, p value of the Chi-square that determines if the classification efficiency is higher than expected by chance. Bold values represent significant differences.

The classification of the calls of captive individuals (Table 3) reinforced the results graphically presented in the PCA (Fig. 4), and indicated that individuals released in Quixadá were released outside their region of origin. Nevertheless, given different possible locations for releasing those individuals, Quixadá was actually the most appropriate locality – for in 48% of the cases it showed the smallest vocal difference. Group A demonstrated a vocal structure closer to the dialect of the Quixadá population, and 82% of the individuals were classified for that region, while only 18% were classified for Serra do Mel. Group B, on the other hand, used a portion of the acoustic space closer to the Serra do Mel dialect (60%) while the remaining individuals could be divided between the Paramoti (20%) and Quixadá (20%). Nearly half the individuals of Group C were classified for Quixadá (42%) or Paramonti (50%), but only a small portion for Icapuí (8%; Table 3). Overall, 48% of the captive birds were classified for Quixadá, followed by 24% for Paramonti and Serra do Mel, with only 3% for Icapuí.

Table 3.

Multinomial regression classification results for captive groups from CETAS.

Group  Icapuí  Paramoti  Quixadá  Serra do Mel  Total 
82%  18%  11 
20%  20%  60%  10 
8%  50%  42%  12 
Total  3%  24%  48%  24%  33 
Fig. 4.

Principal component analysis built with the flight-call parameters of four wild populations and three captive groups of Cactus Conure, depicting distinct usages of the acoustical space.

(0.29MB).
Discussion

Our data indicated the presence of geographic call variations at regional scales at distances less than 100km. Similar results can be found in the literature, where geographical call variations where reported at similar ranges (Bond and Diamond, 2005; Bradbury et al., 2001; Kleeman and Gilardi, 2005; Reynolds et al., 2010; Wright, 1996). Efficient communication is essential for translocation, reintroduction, and release success (Wright and Dahlin, 2017), as individuals within larger social organizations have greater chances of surviving stress situations (Guerra et al., 2008). In Parrots, good flock social organization is directly reflected in the efficiency of foraging, resource acquisition, migratory movements, and anti-predatory behavior (Brightsmith et al., 2005). In that sense, geographic dislocations can negatively influence reintroduction efficiencies as vocal differences can hinder social interactions and preclude proper flock organization, thus reducing the success of reintroduction protocols (Bradbury et al., 2001; Wright and Dahlin, 2017).

Ideally, reintroductions should be made within the original locality of the specimen. However, determining the exact origins of individuals from animal trafficking apprehensions is almost impossible because they typically occur along trade routes, far from the region where the individuals were captured and little information is retrieved from arrested traffickers. Even though efforts have been recently made to determine the geographical origins of individuals through the use of bioacoustics (Magroski et al., 2017), it failed to do so within species with learned calls, such as Parrots. We also failed. The success of the method presented here to determine the geographical origins of recovered specimens would depend on a herculean sampling of the species geographical variation – samples from every single dormitory of the species along its distribution – what would be virtually impossible. Only then we would have the necessary information to determine, with reasonable certainty, the geographical origins of the specimens. In fact, this was not our main objective here.

Under such a scenario of methodological shortfall, immediate actions that could reduce the negative effects of translocations would be very welcome, especially until a permanent solution is available. Our results allowed for the definition of areas with similar vocal dialects, making it possible to reduce overall vocal differences between reintroduced birds and native populations. Group A, for example, in which 82% of the calls were classified for Quixadá, had vocalizations structurally similar to the wild birds at that locality (even thought calls seems more similar to the calls of B and C). Releasing them at Quixadá and not Paramonti should therefore increase their chances of successful adaptation, reducing the potential negative effects of vocal differences. On the other hand, only a few individuals of Group B were classified for Quixadá, so that releasing those individuals in that area would not be the best option. In the case of Psittaciformes, which are known to show great dependence on vocal communication in shaping social and cultural ties, considerations of geographical call variations is extremely important (Guerra et al., 2008), and reducing the vocal differences should improve the efficiency of the releasing process (Wright and Dahlin, 2017).

Everyday realities at CETAS installations scattered throughout Brazil oblige them to release many groups of birds with distinct repertoires in a single locality. Even though the present logistical limitations of CETAS makes such an approach unavoidable, it is important to note that 52% of the released individuals were classified for areas other than Quixadá. By using the methodology presented here it would have been possible to classify those individuals prior to their release and to choose more suitable locations based on the geographical variations of their calls.

We have shown that the Cactus Conure possesses geographical variations on their calls (dialects) throughout its distribution range. The high degree of variation observed between calls ∼70km apart precludes them for large-scale determinations of the geographical origins of captured specimens, but comparing the vocalizations of individuals to be released with vocalizations of possible releasing areas could reduce vocal differences and improve the overall success of returning them to nature. We still lack data, however, about how vocal differences can interfere with socialization processes and affect post-release success. It is known that parrots are open-ended learners, capable of learning throughout their lives (Salinas-Melgoza and Wright, 2012), so that the long-term consequences of releasing specimens with distinct vocal repertoires are still unclear.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Brazilian Institute of Environment and Renewable Natural Resources (IBAMA-CE) for their permission and kind attention while this work was being carried out in the CETAS (Wild Animals Screening Center) at Fortaleza. We would like to thank the two anonymous reviewers for their suggestions and comments. We are also grateful to Lucas Barros for providing sound recordings of wild individuals of Areia Branca, and Flávio Torres for allowing access to his farm at Quixadá. Bruno Martins and Giovanna Rodrigues were supported by CAPES Masters Fellowships, while Carlos B. de Araújo was supported by a CAPES-PNPD Post Doc Fellowship.

References
[Alves et al., 2016]
M.M. Alves, S. de Faria Lopes, R.R.N. Alves.
Wild vertebrates kept as pets in the semiarid region of Brazil.
Trop. Conserv. Sci., 9 (2016), pp. 354-368
[Berg et al., 2011]
K.S. Berg, S. Delgado, R. Okawa, S.R. Beissinger, J.W. Bradbury.
Contact calls are used for individual mate recognition in free-ranging green-rumped parrotlets, Forpus passerinus.
Anim. Behav., 81 (2011), pp. 241-248
[Berkunsky et al., 2017]
I. Berkunsky, P. Quillfeldt, D.J. Brightsmith, M.C. Abbud, J.M.R.E. Aguilar, U. Alemán-Zelaya, R.M. Aramburú, A. Arce Arias, R. Balas McNab, T.J.S. Balsby, J.M. Barredo Barberena, S.R. Beissinger, M. Rosales, K.S. Berg, C.A. Bianchi, E. Blanco, A. Bodrati, C. Bonilla-Ruz, E. Botero-Delgadillo, S.B. Canavelli, R. Caparroz, R.E. Cepeda, O. Chassot, C. Cinta-Magallón, K.L. Cockle, G. Daniele, C.B. de Araujo, A.E. de Barbosa, L.N. de Moura, H. Del Castillo, S. Díaz, J.A. Díaz-Luque, L. Douglas, A. Figueroa Rodríguez, R.A. García-Anleu, J.D. Gilardi, P.G. Grilli, J.C. Guix, M. Hernández, A. Hernández-Muñoz, F. Hiraldo, E. Horstman, R. Ibarra Portillo, J.P. Isacch, J.E. Jiménez, L. Joyner, M. Juarez, F.P. Kacoliris, V.T. Kanaan, L. Klemann-Júnior, S.C. Latta, A.T.K. Lee, A. Lesterhuis, M. Lezama-López, C. Lugarini, G. Marateo, C.B. Marinelli, J. Martínez, M.S. McReynolds, C.R. Mejia Urbina, G. Monge-Arias, T.C. Monterrubio-Rico, A.P. Nunes, F. Nunes, C. Olaciregui, J. Ortega-Arguelles, E. Pacifico, L. Pagano, N. Politi, G. Ponce-Santizo, H.O. Portillo Reyes, N.P. Prestes, F. Presti, K. Renton, G. Reyes-Macedo, E. Ringler, L. Rivera, A. Rodríguez-Ferraro, A.M. Rojas-Valverde, R.E. Rojas-Llanos, Y.G. Rubio-Rocha, A.B.S. Saidenberg, A. Salinas-Melgoza, V. Sanz, H.M. Schaefer, P. Scherer-Neto, G.H.F. Seixas, P. Serafini, L.F. Silveira, E.A.B. Sipinski, M. Somenzari, D. Susanibar, J.L. Tella, C. Torres-Sovero, C. Trofino-Falasco, R. Vargas-Rodríguez, L.D. Vázquez-Reyes, T.H. White, S. Williams, R. Zarza, J.F. Masello.
Current threats faced by Neotropical parrot populations.
Biol. Conserv., 214 (2017), pp. 278-287
[Bond and Diamond, 2005]
A.B. Bond, J. Diamond.
Geographic and ontogenetic variation in the contact calls of the kea (Nestor notabilis).
Behaviour, 142 (2005), pp. 1-20
[Bradbury and Balsby, 2016]
J.W. Bradbury, T.J.S. Balsby.
The functions of vocal learning in parrots.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 70 (2016), pp. 293-312
[Bradbury et al., 2001]
J.W. Bradbury, K.A. Cortopassi, J.R. Clemmons.
Geographical variation in the contact calls of orange-fronted parakeets.
Auk, 118 (2001), pp. 958-972
[Brightsmith et al., 2005]
D. Brightsmith, J. Hilburn, A. Del Campo, J. Boyd, M. Frisius, R. Frisius, D. Janik, F. Guillen.
The use of hand-raised psittacines for reintroduction: a case study of scarlet macaws (Ara macao) in Peru and Costa Rica.
Biol. Conserv., 121 (2005), pp. 465-472
[Burnham et al., 2011]
K.P. Burnham, D.R. Anderson, K.P. Huyvaert.
AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 65 (2011), pp. 23-35
[Collar, 2]
N.J. Collar.
Globally threatened parrots: criteria, characteristics and cures.
Int. Zoo Yearb., 37 (2000), pp. 21-35
[Daut et al., 2015]
E.F. Daut, D.J. Brightsmith, A.P. Mendoza, L. Puhakka, M.J. Peterson.
Illegal domestic bird trade and the role of export quotas in Peru.
J. Nat. Conserv., 27 (2015), pp. 44-53
[de Araújo, 2011]
C.B. de Araújo.
Psitacídeos do Cerrado: Sua alimentação, comunicação sonora e aspectos bióticos e abióticos de sua distribuição potencial.
UNICAMP, (2011),
[de Araújo et al., 2011]
C.B. de Araújo, L.O. Marcondes-Machado, J. Vielliard.
Vocal repertoire of the yellow-faced parrot (Alipiopsitta xanthops).
Wilson J. Ornithol., 123 (2011), pp. 603-608
[de Moura et al., 2011]
L.N. de Moura, M.L. Silva, J. Vielliard.
Vocal repertoire of wild breeding Orange-winged Parrots Amazona amazonica in Amazonia.
Bioacoustics, 20 (2011), pp. 331-339
[Defoe, 1719]
D. Defoe.
The Life and Strange Surprizing Adventures of Robinson Crusoe, of York, Mariner.
W. Taylor, (1719),
[Destro et al., 2012]
G.F.G. Destro, R. Barreto, R.M. Sabaini, R.C. Borges, T.L. Pimentel.
Efforts to Combat Wild Animals Trafficking in Brazil.
INTECH Open Access Publisher, (2012),
[Fernandes-Ferreira et al., 2012]
H. Fernandes-Ferreira, S.V. Mendonça, C. Albano, F.S. Ferreira, R.R.N. Alves.
Hunting, use and conservation of birds in Northeast Brazil.
Biodivers. Conserv., 21 (2012), pp. 221-244
[Fernandes and Caparroz, 2013]
G.A. Fernandes, R. Caparroz.
DNA sequence analysis to guide the release of blue-and-yellow macaws (Ara ararauna, Psittaciformes, Aves) from the illegal trade back into the wild.
Mol. Biol. Rep., 40 (2013), pp. 2757-2762
[Fernandez-Juricic et al., 1998]
E. Fernandez-Juricic, M.B. Martella, E.V. Alvarez.
Vocalizations of the blue-fronted amazon (Amazona aestiva) in the Chancani Reserve, Cordoba, Argentina.
Wilson Bull., 103 (1998), pp. 352-361
[Forshaw, 2010]
J.M. Forshaw.
Parrots of the World.
Princeton University Press, (2010),
[Gridi-Papp, 2007]
M. Gridi-Papp.
SoundRuler: Acoustic Analysis for Research and Teaching.
[Guerra et al., 2008]
J.E. Guerra, J. Cruz-Nieto, S.G. Ortiz-Maciel, T.F. Wright.
Limited geographic variation in the vocalizations of the endangered thick-billed parrot: implications for conservation strategies.
Condor, 110 (2008), pp. 639-647
[IUCN, 2000]
IUCN.
Guidelines for the Placement of Confiscated Animals.
Aprovado no 51° Encontro do Conselho da IUCN, (2000),
(WWW document)
[Jarvis et al., 2000]
E.D. Jarvis, S. Ribeiro, M.L. Silva, D. Ventura, J. Vielliard, C.V. Mello.
Behaviourally driven gene expression reveals song nuclei in hummingbird brain.
Nature, 406 (2000), pp. 628-632
[Kleeman and Gilardi, 2005]
P.M. Kleeman, J.D. Gilardi.
Geographical variation of St. Lucia parrot flight vocalizations.
Condor, 107 (2005), pp. 62-68
[Lachlan and Servedio, 2004]
R.F. Lachlan, M.R. Servedio.
Song learning accelerates allopatric speciation.
Evolution (NY), 58 (2004), pp. 2049-2063
[Magroski et al., 2017]
L.M. Magroski, A. do Nascimento Pessoa, W.G. de Lucena, A. Loures-Ribeiro, C.B. de Araújo.
Where to release birds seized from illegal traffic? The value of vocal analyses and ecological niche modeling.
Perspect. Ecol. Conserv., 15 (2017), pp. 91-101
[Marini and Garcia, 2005]
M.A. Marini, F.I. Garcia.
Bird conservation in Brazil.
Conserv. Biol., 19 (2005), pp. 665-671
[Mazerolle, 2011]
M.J. Mazerolle.
AICcmodavg: Model Selection and Multimodel Inference Based on (Q) AIC (c). R Packag. Version 1.
(2011),
[McGregor et al., 1992]
P.K. McGregor, C.K. Catchpole, T. Dabelsteen, J.B. Falls, L. Fusani, H.C. Gerhardt, F. Gilbert, A.G. Horn, G.M. Klump, D.E. Kroodsma.
Design of playback experiments: the Thornbridge Hall NATO ARW consensus.
Playback and Studies of Animal Communication, Springer, (1992), pp. 1-9
[Medina-García et al., 2015]
A. Medina-García, M. Araya-Salas, T.F. Wright.
Does vocal learning accelerate acoustic diversification? Evolution of contact calls in Neotropical parrots.
J. Evol. Biol., 28 (2015), pp. 1782-1792
[Nobrega Alves et al., 2013]
R.R. Nobrega Alves, J.R. De Farias Lima, H.F.P. Araujo.
The live bird trade in Brazil and its conservation implications: an overview.
Bird Conserv. Int., 23 (2013), pp. 53-65
[Presti et al., 2015]
F.T. Presti, N.M.R. Guedes, P.T.Z. Antas, C.Y. Miyaki.
Population genetic structure in hyacinth macaws (Anodorhynchus hyacinthinus) and identification of the probable origin of confiscated individuals.
J. Hered., 106 (2015), pp. 491-502
[R Core Team, 2015]
R Core Team.
R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Internet].
R Foundation for Statistical Computing, (2015),
Doc. Free available internet http://www.r-project.org
[Reynolds et al., 2010]
M.B.J. Reynolds, W.K. Hayes, J.W. Wiley.
Geographic variation in the flight call of the Cuban Parrot (Amazona leucocephala) and its taxonomic relevance.
J. Caribb. Ornithol., 23 (2010), pp. 4-18
[Ripley and Venables, 2011]
B. Ripley, W. Venables.
nnet: Feed-Forward Neural Networks and Multinomial Log-linear Models. R Packag. Version 7.
(2011),
[Salinas-Melgoza and Wright, 2012]
A. Salinas-Melgoza, T.F. Wright.
Evidence for vocal learning and limited dispersal as dual mechanisms for dialect maintenance in a parrot.
PLoS ONE, 7 (2012), pp. e48667
[Sewall et al., 2016]
K.B. Sewall, A.M. Young, T.F. Wright.
Social calls provide novel insights into the evolution of vocal learning.
Anim. Behav., 120 (2016), pp. 163-172
[Sick, 1997]
H. Sick.
Ornitologia Brasileira.
Nov. Front., (1997),
[Vielliard and Silva, 2010]
J.M.E. Vielliard, M.L. Silva.
A Bioacústica como ferramenta de pesquisa em Comportamento animal.
Estudos Do Comportamento Animal II, pp. 145-160
[Wanker et al., 2005]
R. Wanker, Y. Sugama, S. Prinage.
Vocal labelling of family members in spectacled parrotlets, Forpus conspicillatus.
Anim. Behav., 70 (2005), pp. 111-118
[Wright, 1996]
T.F. Wright.
Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B: Biol. Sci., 263 (1996), pp. 867-872
[Wright and Dahlin, 2017]
T.F. Wright, C.R. Dahlin.
Vocal dialects in parrots: patterns and processes of cultural evolution.
Emu-Austral Ornithol., (2017), pp. 1-17
[Wright et al., 2008]
T.F. Wright, C.R. Dahlin, A. Salinas-Melgoza.
Stability and change in vocal dialects of the yellow-naped amazon.
Anim. Behav., 76 (2008), pp. 1017-1027
[Zollinger et al., 2012]
S.A. Zollinger, J. Podos, E. Nemeth, F. Goller, H. Brumm.
On the relationship between, and measurement of, amplitude and frequency in birdsong.
Anim. Behav., 84 (2012), pp. e1-e9
[Zuur et al., 2010]
A.F. Zuur, E.N. Ieno, C.S. Elphick.
A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems.
Methods Ecol. Evol., 1 (2010), pp. 3-14
Copyright © 2018. Associação Brasileira de Ciência Ecológica e Conservação
Download PDF
Perspectives in Ecology and Conservation
Article options
Tools